% Mastery Mailing 1 \documentclass[12pt]{apa7} \usepackage[style=apa,backend=biber]{biblatex} \usepackage{graphicx} \setlength{\headheight}{15pt} % According to several sources, the following commands should be active % for an APA paper, but I just hate them. % \raggedright % \language255 % no hyphenation \parindent=.5in \linespread{2} \shorttitle{Article Review III} \addbibresource{sources.bib} \leftheader{Rohrer} \begin{document} \centerline{\textbf{Article Review III: Who's the ``Real'' Victim? Victim Framing and Sexual Assault}} Public opinion on rape cases continues to affect high-profile allegations like those in the \#MeToo movement or Brett Kavanaugh's case. Therefore, it's very important for public communicators (like reporters) to understand the effect from implicitly biased wording on their readers. This study, ``Who's the `Real' Victim,'' studies a rhetorical device called ``victim framing'' and how people's opinions differ over a case depending on the news they read. The researchers ran the study from several samples of Amazon's Mechanical Turk service, obtaining a ``sample of convenience.'' About 2400 people participated in the study across four experiments. The first three of these experiments asked participants about a fictional rape case on a college campus, framed either neutrally (to create a baseline metric for opinions on sexual assault), framed with the accused as the victim, or framed with the assaulted as the victim. The framing were transparently anecdotal quotes attributed to friends of the protagonists, saying ``[he/she] is the real victim here'' \autocite{assault}. The article samples presented to participants also vary on the amount of detail included (sparse vs rich descriptions of the case and campus opinions). This examines how important the level of elaboration is on persuading participants from their originally-held beliefs. The second experiment asked participants to cite the part of the text that affected their opinions of the case most. There was an observed significant interaction between people citing the quote describing victimhood and being swayed by the argument. The third experiment used very sparse language to frame its protagonist as a victim. The expansive victimhood arguments more consistently persuaded participants to lean on their beliefs, but even the very sparse descriptions mentioning one protagonist or the other as a victim biased readers. The fourth experiment was the true example of Brett Kavanaugh's hearing using the same text as the fictional case observed in previous trials. This trial was conducted about 10 months after his hearings. Victimhood language was less impactful to readers in the real case, but some significant effects appeared. The independent variables measured were level of detail in the story, level of detail in the victimhood statement, and the truth of the story. The dependent variable measured was Likert-scale self-report sympathy to the assaulted protagonist or to the accused protagonist, and (in some experiments) whether the reader cited language about victimhood as impactful in their decision. The study relates itself to existing theory about how arguments convince people called social-pragmatic reasoning. This is where biased language (like saying a basketball player ``misses 60\%'' or ``makes 40\%'' of their shots) causes a reader to assume the author has a good reason to write that way. This inference-forming method means calling a protagonist a victim may activate a ``dyadic account of moral reasoning'' \autocite{assault}. Judging a person as a ``moral agent or patient'' in a situation causes observers to reduce blame for a protagonist seen as a passive actor (in contrast to the increased responsibility for a protagonist perceived as an agent). The authors controlled for demand characteristics in this study by portraying themselves as trying to learn public opinion on a report. This study was the first to confirm ``victim framing'' as a potent way to affect public opinion, but the results from the real case show it may not be so reliable. People who did not cite the victimhood statement as cementing their opinion had less sympathy for Kavanaugh when he was treated as the victim. This is probably a backfire effect against deeply-held beliefs because this population was much more likely to hold liberal beliefs, and therefore already have little sympathy towards Kavanaugh. Despite the issues this study has for generalization---it was conducted on Mechanical Turk, so it doesn't have a very representative sample---this study has implications for real-world reporting. Victim-framing appears, for example, when the {\it Washington Times} published the article ``Christine Blasey Ford is not the victim here---Brett Kavanaugh is.'' Further research is still required on how exactly victim framing convinces people, but since it does have an impact, we need to decide on policy to handle this issue. \iffalse - Hypothesis - IV/DV - Controls - Results - Conclusions \fi \vfil\eject \printbibliography \end{document}